I first read Daniel Stern’s The Interpersonal World of
the Infant as a trainee in about 1989.
It was a new book, hot off the presses, and it questioned many of the
assumptions that the people who were training me had held dear for most of
their careers. And, much to my surprise,
these very talented and capable people were excited – instead of threatened –
by the book and the ideas in it. I think
they were excited by it – and this is purely speculative – because this book
was consistent with their experience of their own children in the ways that the
previous psychoanalytic theories had not been.
Whether or not that was the case, it is the case that this dense book,
written in a very high tone, but with good examples, helped me greet my son in
a way that seemed to me to be consonant with who he was when he was born. That said, I also need to clarify that he was
born ten years later, which is probably a good thing, because it took a
considerable time for the material from this book to sink in – and frankly,
re-reading it every year as I do now for a graduate course that I teach, I find
new things in it each time that I read it.
This book seeks to integrate two very different
literatures. One is the psychoanalytic
literature that is based on the “re-created infant” – a child remembered in the
therapeutic process – sometimes by the adult (in conversation with his or her
analyst) on the couch – sometimes from working with young children and trying
to imagine, based on newly verbal kids, what the internal experience of
pre-verbal infants must be like. The
other is the growing literature that Stern tapped into (and contributed to) about
the “observed infant” – a preverbal infant that a researcher “asks” to answer
questions about his or her internal states having the infant do the things that
the infant is able to do. A newborn, for
instance, is able to move his or her eyes to track things, is able to turn his
or her head when it is supported, and is able to engage in sucking behavior. So, we use these few things that newborns can
do to ask them questions. We ask them –
can they remember? We bring in their
mother’s breast pad and place it on one side of their head and the breast pad
from another woman and place it on the other side, and they reliably turn their
heads towards their mother’s breast pad.
They have memory (and other faculties) right from the get-go.
More central to his thesis, we give infants a pacifier that is
either smooth or bumpy. We let them suck
on it without seeing it. We then show
them two pacifiers – one that is bumpy and one that is smooth – and they spend
more time looking at the one that is like the one they have been sucking
on. This turns out to be a tremendously
important piece of information. What the
infants are communicating to us is that they can transform tactile information –
what the pacifier feels like – into an abstract representation – what it is
shaped like – that they can recognize visually.
This ability – to represent something concrete in abstract terms – again
essentially from birth – turns our understanding of the mind of the infant
upside down. Instead of having to learn
all kinds of intermediate steps to build up to being able to represent
something perceived in their minds, they can do this essentially
immediately. OK, so we have to rethink
Piaget and rework our understanding of cognitive development. But Stern doesn’t really get all that
interested in this momentous shift – he is much more interested in why we are equipped to do that.
Stern spends the rest of the book articulating that the
reason we are built to represent things cross modally is because this supports our
ability to relate to our caregivers – and this, in turn, facilitates our
learning lots of things from them – but primarily how to relate. To get from here to there, Stern notes that
we are not born with an oceanic lack of knowledge about where we begin and end,
but actually we learn quite quickly that we have a boundedness and we
experience Mommy as a separate entity in the world (this is radically different
from some psychoanalytic developmental schemas). He also introduces the notion of vitality
affects – the idea that emotions are not just categorically different – but that
they come in a rush – that they may start out slow and then become stronger –
or they may taper off slowly. He uses
this fact to note that we use the vitality component to communicate to infants
that we know what they are feeling by matching the speed of what we are doing
to the speed of what they are doing – that we can mirror what the infant is
doing. But, and this is the really cool
part, we don’t just mirror, we exploit the infant’s ability to represent things
cross modally, to make a sound that we emit track the intensity of their
muscular tension – and, in the next moment, we open our eyes as wide as the
sound they emit.
What Stern is observing are the everyday interactions that
take place between infants and parents.
As a result of that observation, he is able to articulate what we as a
species know intuitively with the more precise language of science. We can communicate feeling states with each
other and this is the basis of human communication. What he adds is the postulate that we are
built to do just that. We are built to
communicate. He then notes that the
culmination of this communication ability – the introduction of language –
learning to talk – is a two edged sword.
We are able to communicate particular thoughts more accurately, but at
the cost of a loss of affective communication.
The richness of our communication actually plummets as the precision
increases. Our infants become, in a
weird but very palpable way, less interesting as they learn to talk.
So, with Stern (and, frankly, T. Berry Brazelton, a
pediatrician who writes about these phenomena in much more accessible terms), I
was able to greet my son in his early life not just intuitively, but as a true
geek should, with book learning. We
spent time together playing, hanging out, and napping. I thought that I would get a lot accomplished
when he was napping, but I rarely did. I
usually napped with him – being fully engaged is hard work – for the kid and
(at least this) parent. My experience of
our time together was of connecting with a huge intelligence – one that was
curious about the world, about me, about himself – and wanting to learn more
about it. So I was terribly disappointed
when he learned to talk (Oh, don’t get me wrong – I was excited – you would
think no person had ever learned to talk before – I was amazed and proud about
this incredible achievement), I was disappointed that the imagined other – the kid
that was so broadly curious – now would utter one word statements – “Cookie” –
that collapsed all of that wonderful space that we had built between us. Language is a double edged sword indeed!
Of course, as Stern points out, we learn to use language to create cross modal descriptions - they are the basis of poetry - my love is like a red, red rose. This, in turn, facilitates the kinds of communications that can be lost when language first appears - though the functions of language are manifold and they can move us away from an intersubjectivity as well as move us towards it.
Of course, as Stern points out, we learn to use language to create cross modal descriptions - they are the basis of poetry - my love is like a red, red rose. This, in turn, facilitates the kinds of communications that can be lost when language first appears - though the functions of language are manifold and they can move us away from an intersubjectivity as well as move us towards it.
Stern has a bigger ax to grind in this book. He is using his evidence to support an
intersubjective theory of psychoanalysis.
I think there is something to this argument. But I also think that this developmental
perspective can help inform many other psychoanalytic theories as well. For instance, many older psychoanalytic
theories postulate a diffuse, boundary less state as a part of normal
development – and suggest that later boundarylessness is a regression to an
earlier state. While it may be that the
later state is a regression to an earlier one – it may be a regression to a pathological earlier state of
boundarilessness – perhaps related to early traumatic experiences that crossed
personal boundaries, overwhelming the infant’s capacity to manage the
autonomous state they were built to inhabit.
But it is not just in theory that this book can help. In our “technique” – how it is that we
connect with others, this can be a truly useful perspective. As we better understand the actual infant, we
will be better able to connect, not just with those infants (this really did
help me in raising a child), but with the adults that they become. Adults who have amazing capacities for
communication – for communion really – can see these capacities atrophy as they
focus on developing other skills – such us finding just the right words to communicate
how an infant develops. Not all is lost,
however. When we play with infants, we
regain a sense of how spectacular it is to be in contact with another human
being. Also, when we play in therapy,
we can connect with that “inner child” and remember what it means to feel
authentically – and we can use that connection as a base to move forward in
relating in very adult – deeply felt - adult ways.
To access a narrative description of other posts on this site, link here. For a subject based index, link here.
To access a narrative description of other posts on this site, link here. For a subject based index, link here.
To subscribe to posts (which occur 2-3 times per month), if you are on a computer, hit the X button on the upper right of this screen and, on the subsequent screen, hover your cursor over the black line in the upper right area and choose the pop out box that says subscribe and then enter the information. I'm sorry but I don't currently know how you can subscribe from a mobile device - hopefully you have a computer as well...
No comments:
Post a Comment